What are the three components of memory according to Atkinson and Shiffrin?

A model of memory proposed by Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) that features memory as being comprised of three major components: sensory memory, short-term memory, and long-term memory. Also known as the Multistore Model of Memory.

Introduction

In their proposal of the Dual-Store Model, Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) claim that memory processing involves a series of sequential processes that lead to the input, storage and usage of information. This model of memory breaks down memory processing into three distinct components known as sensory memory, short-term memory, and long-term memory. Information is first processed in the form of sensory memory, and subsequent processing leads this information to be stored in short-term memory and later transferred to long-term memory.

Sensory Memory

In the initial phase of memory processing, stimuli from the environment are detected via the senses and the information provided is stored in the form of sensory memory. The existence of such a...

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

References

  • Atkinson, R., & Shiffrin, R. (1968). Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 2, 89–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0079-7421(08)60422-3.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1971). The control of short-term memory. Scientific American, 225(2), 82–90. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0871-82.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Averbach, E., & Coriell, A. S. (1961). Short-term memory in vision. Bell System Technical Journal, 40(1), 309–328. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1538-7305.1961.tb03987.x.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1966). The influence of acoustic and semantic similarity on long-term memory for word sequences. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(4), 302–309. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640746608400047.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47–89.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1976). Verbal reasoning and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28(4), 603–621.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 55(1), 75–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1964.tb00899.x.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671–684. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(72)80001-x.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Glanzer, M., & Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5(4), 351–360. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-5371(66)80044-0.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043158.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B. (1959). Perceptual and memory disturbances in human temporal lesions. Acta Psychologica, 15, 217–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0001-6918(59)80089-5.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B. (1965). Physiologie de l’Hippocampe. Neuropsychologia, 3(3), 273–277. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(65)90029-1.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H. (1968). Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M. Neuropsychologia, 6(3), 215–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(68)90021-3.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(5), 482–488. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045106.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20(1), 11–21. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11.

    CrossRef  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shallice, T., & Warrington, E. K. (1974). The dissociation between short term retention of meaningful sounds and verbal material. Neuropsychologia, 12(4), 553–555.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. M., Glenberg, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1978). Environmental context and human memory. Memory and Cognition, 6(4), 342–353. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03197465.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. (1960). Negative afterimage without prior positive image. Science, 131(3413), 1613–1614. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.131.3413.1613.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. (1963). A model for visual memory tasks. Human Factors: Journal of Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 5(1), 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872086300500103.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–402). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020071.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, New York, USA

    Russell A. Vogel

Authors

  1. Russell A. Vogel

    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Russell A. Vogel .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

  1. Oakland University , Rochester, Michigan, USA

    Jennifer Vonk

  2. Department of Psychology, Oakland University Department of Psychology, Rochester, Michigan, USA

    Ph.D. Todd Shackelford

Section Editor information

  1. Hofstra University, Long Island, USA

    Oskar Pineno

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this entry

Cite this entry

Vogel, R.A. (2017). Dual-Store Model. In: Vonk, J., Shackelford, T. (eds) Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1228-1

What are the 3 stages of the Atkinson Shiffrin model of memory?

In order for a memory to go into storage (i.e., long-term memory), it has to pass through three distinct stages: Sensory Memory, Short-Term Memory, and finally Long-Term Memory. These stages were first proposed by Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin (1968).

What is the 3 component model of memory?

It describes the general nature of the three components (sensory register, working memory, long-term memory) and integrates the roles of attention, in-depth cognitive processing, and executive functions into the model.

What did Atkinson and Shiffrin say about memory?

Atkinson and Shiffrin believed that once information enters the brain, it must be either stored or maintained and that the information which is stored goes into three distinct memory systems: the sensory register, short-term memory, and long-term memory.